Featured image for blog post: Bias and Confounding in Plain Language. Simple explanations and safeguards for observational research.

Bias and Confounding in Plain Language

August 8, 2025

7 min read 1.5k words
Health Economics & Outcomes Research outcomes researchreal-world evidencehealthcare

Observational studies use data from everyday care to learn what works outside a trial. That is powerful—and tricky. Without randomization, differences between people and care settings can distort results. The good news: a handful of design habits and transparent checks catch most problems. If you are new to the overall landscape, start with the overview of real‑world evidence in healthcare decision‑making to see where observational studies fit. And ground your work in outcomes that matter using the checklist in measuring health outcomes that matter.

What “bias” and “confounding” mean in practice

  • Bias means a systematic tilt in the result. The estimate is off in a particular direction, not just randomly noisy.
  • Confounding means a third factor is linked to both the treatment and the outcome, creating a misleading association.

Picture a postpartum outreach program offered more often to higher‑risk patients. If outcomes improve, was it the program—or the fact that higher‑risk patients also received more clinical attention? Careful design and analysis are needed to separate the signal from the noise.

The most common design traps (plain language)

  1. Confounding by indication
  • What it is: People receive a therapy because they are sicker or because clinicians think they will benefit more. Sickness severity then drives outcomes.
  • Example: A new antihypertensive is given to postpartum patients with the highest blood pressures. Worse outcomes in that group may reflect baseline risk, not drug harm.
  • Safeguards: Use an active comparator (another antihypertensive). Adjust for baseline blood pressure and related factors. Consider a “new‑user” cohort that starts therapy after a clean window.
  1. Immortal time bias
  • What it is: A built‑in period where the outcome cannot occur for one group because they haven’t yet qualified for exposure.
  • Example: Counting outcomes from delivery, but defining exposure as “received home BP monitor in the first 14 days.” People who got a monitor must have survived and not had a severe event for up to 14 days, biasing results in their favor.
  • Safeguards: Start the clock (time zero) when exposure status is known for both groups, or use time‑varying exposure.
  1. Selection bias
  • What it is: Who gets into the study differs in ways related to outcomes.
  • Example: Including only patients with portal accounts to study a digital follow‑up tool excludes people with access barriers.
  • Safeguards: Define inclusion criteria based on routine data every patient has. Report coverage and compare participants to the wider eligible population.
  1. Misclassification (measurement bias)
  • What it is: Exposure or outcome is recorded incorrectly.
  • Example: Using a single blood pressure reading from a flawed device; or an outcome that relies on a code some clinics never use.
  • Safeguards: Build robust phenotypes (multiple codes, labs, time rules). Validate a sample by chart review; publish positive predictive value (PPV).
  1. Time‑window bias
  • What it is: Groups have different lengths of follow‑up or observation windows.
  • Example: Comparing 30‑day outcomes for one therapy to 90‑day outcomes for another.
  • Safeguards: Freeze windows up front; use the same windows across groups.
  1. Collider bias (conditioning on a consequence)
  • What it is: Selecting on a factor that is caused by both exposure and outcome distorts associations.
  • Example: Studying only people who attend follow‑up visits can create spurious links between treatment and outcome because visit attendance depends on both.
  • Safeguards: Avoid conditioning on post‑exposure variables unless using methods that handle them explicitly.
  1. Regression to the mean
  • What it is: Extreme values tend to move toward the average on repeat measurement.
  • Example: Enrolling patients after a spike in blood pressure will show improvement even without treatment changes.
  • Safeguards: Use repeated baseline measures and active comparators; focus on clinically meaningful endpoints, not only raw values.

Design first: habits that prevent problems

Adopt a “target trial” mindset from the start—define the trial you wish you could run, then emulate it with real‑world data. See practical scaffolding in designing real‑world evidence studies that matter and from RCTs to RWE: bridging the evidence gap.

Key habits:

  • Define time zero clearly. Align exposure assignment and follow‑up start.
  • Use new‑user designs when feasible (exclude prevalent users to avoid history effects).
  • Choose active comparators that reflect realistic choices.
  • Freeze outcome definitions and windows in plain English. Publish a short glossary.
  • Pre‑specify primary/secondary outcomes and subgroup plans. Keep a change log.

Quality of inputs matters as much as design. Map data flows and run basic checks—completeness, consistency, plausibility, and timeliness—using the playbook in EHR data quality for real‑world evidence. Publish “data notes” with known limits.

Analysis: balancing apples to apples (without hiding the ball)

Observational studies do not have to be opaque. Prefer methods that stakeholders can understand and you can explain in two sentences.

  • Propensity scores: summarize many covariates into a single score for matching or weighting. Show covariate balance before and after.
  • Inverse‑probability weighting: weight people by the inverse of how likely they were to receive the treatment they actually got, given baseline factors.
  • Doubly robust estimators: combine weighting and outcome models; retain protection if one is misspecified.
  • Fixed‑effects or difference‑in‑differences: compare changes over time across groups when staggered adoption occurs.
  • Instrumental variables: use with caution and clear justification; valid instruments are rare.

Always publish the covariate list with clinical rationale. Show standardized differences and overlap (common support). If overlap is poor, do not force a comparison; narrow the question instead.

Sensitivity checks that build trust

Simple, pre‑planned checks go a long way:

  • Negative controls: outcomes unrelated to the exposure should show no effect.
  • Falsification tests: exposures that should not affect the outcome (e.g., a different time window) should show no effect.
  • Alternative windows and definitions: ensure findings are not brittle.
  • Missing‑data strategy: state what was imputed, how, and how results changed.
  • Subgroup consistency: results should not flip wildly across reasonable subgroups unless you have a strong mechanism.

When decisions are near‑term and visible, present results as a concise brief using the structure in AI‑assisted evidence synthesis for policy briefs.

Equity and fairness: measure, don’t assume

Every comparative result should include subgroup views by language, race/ethnicity (when collected), payer, and neighborhood. Report:

  • Coverage: who is in the sample vs. who is eligible
  • Precision: stability of estimates in each subgroup
  • Calibration (for prediction): alignment between predicted and observed risk

If gaps appear, consider design remedies (better comparators, more complete covariates) and operational ones (interpreter‑first workflows, flexible hours). For outreach‑linked programs, adopt the safeguards in AI for population health management.

Case vignette 1: postpartum home blood‑pressure monitoring

Question: Does providing home BP cuffs with interpreter‑first outreach reduce severe postpartum hypertension events within 10 days?

  • Design: target trial emulation; new‑user cohort starting at discharge; active comparator is usual discharge without a cuff.
  • Confounding risks: indication (higher‑risk patients receive cuffs); immortal time bias if time zero is delivery.
  • Safeguards: time zero is discharge for both groups; adjust for baseline BP, parity, and comorbidities; capacity‑matched lists for outreach.
  • Result: day‑10 checks rise to 67%; severe events fall by 24%. Subgroup analysis shows larger gains among patients with interpreter need, consistent with programs described in AI for registries and quality improvement and workflows in AI for population health management.

Case vignette 2: device safety signal after launch

Question: Are 7‑day ED visits higher post‑procedure compared with an established device?

  • Design: active‑comparator new‑user cohort; registry linked to EHR/claims.
  • Bias risks: selection (higher‑complexity sites adopt earlier), misclassification (device codes), time‑window mismatch.
  • Safeguards: funnel plots by site; robust device identifiers; matched windows; weighting with overlap diagnostics. See monitoring patterns in real‑world safety monitoring after launch.

Case vignette 3: claims‑based injury prevention

Question: Did protected bike lanes reduce pedestrian/cyclist ED visits near target intersections?

  • Design: difference‑in‑differences using matched control intersections.
  • Bias risks: coding changes over time; spillover effects.
  • Safeguards: track code changes and annotate trend breaks; sensitivity analyses with alternative buffers. For indicator design and validation, see using claims data for injury prevention.

Reporting: show your work in plain English

Make it easy to see what you did and why.

  • One‑paragraph rationale: who, what, when, and the decision the result informs.
  • A short glossary of definitions and windows.
  • A table or appendix listing covariates and balance metrics.
  • Charts with annotations when definitions or data sources changed.
  • Equity views and remedies if disparities are detected.

If results will influence coverage or program funding, tie outcomes to value in plain terms (staff time, visits avoided, days at home) and, when appropriate, include both cost‑effectiveness and budget impact—see framing in Health Economics 101 for Clinical Teams.

When observational isn’t enough

Sometimes bias risks remain too large. Elevate to a pragmatic design that adds randomization while staying practical: stepped‑wedge rollouts, registry‑based randomized trials, or point‑of‑care randomization. See options in pragmatic trials and RWE: better together.

Quick checklist

  • Phrase the decision and target trial in plain English.
  • Freeze outcome definitions and windows; publish a glossary.
  • Pick active comparators; prefer new‑user designs; align time zero.
  • Map data flows; run fitness checks; publish data notes.
  • Show covariate lists, balance, and overlap; avoid forced comparisons.
  • Pre‑plan sensitivity checks and subgroup views.
  • Report results as a one‑page brief with equity and next steps.

Key takeaways

  • Clear design beats complex analysis when preventing bias.
  • Most traps have simple, transparent safeguards.
  • Equity requires measurement and remedies, not assumptions.

← Back to all posts